2023 Impact Factor
Bitter taste receptors are expressed throughout the digestive tract. Data on animals have suggested these receptors are involved in the gut hormone release, but no data are available in humans. Our aim is to assess whether bitter agonists influence food intake and gut hormone release in healthy subjects.
Twenty healthy volunteers were enrolled in a double-blind cross-over study. On 2 different days, each subject randomly received an acid-resistant capsule containing either placebo or 18 mg of hydrochloride (HCl) quinine. After 60 minutes, all subjects were allowed to eat an
Calorie intake was significantly lower when subjects received HCl quinine than placebo (514 ± 248 vs 596 ± 286 kcal;
This study showed that intra-duodenal release of a bitter compound is able to significantly affect calorie intake and CCK release after a standardized meal. Our results suggest that bitter taste receptor signaling may have a crucial role in the control of food intake.
Taste is a complex sensory modality involved in the detection of food quality. It is commonly accepted that the so-called “basic” tastes (ie, bitter, sweet, umami, sour, and salty) have been naturally selected to facilitate nutrient consumption and help avoid the ingestion of potentially harmful foods.1 For instance, the sweet taste is perceived as palatable and is classically associated with energy-dense foods, whereas the bitter taste is innately aversive and related to toxic compounds.
The mechanisms underlying taste detection in the oral cavity have been well established; in fact, tastants are recognized by a number of receptors, including ion channels (salty and sour) and G protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs) (bitter, sweet, and umami).1 Specifically, the GPCR type 1 taste receptor family (T1R) comprises 3 different subunits that heterodimerize to detect sweet (T1R2 + T1R3) and umami (T1R1 + T1R3) tastants, while the type 2 taste receptor (T2R) family is responsive to bitter compounds.2,3 The ability to taste bitter compounds varies greatly among individuals. The best-known example of this variability is the genetic ability to taste phenylthiocarbamide (PTC) and 6-N propylthiouracil (PROP). Based on the ability to recognize PTC or PROP, individuals can be defined as “tasters” or “non-tasters.”4
In the last decade, several studies have demonstrated the expression of taste receptors and their downstream signaling molecules in the gut, leading to the hypothesis that gastrointestinal taste system may play a role in the gut chemosensitivity.5,6 In particular, studies in model cell lines7,8 and a histochemical study9 have shown that T2R family members are expressed by entero-endocrine cells (EECs), which constitute a first level of integration of the information coming from the lumen.10 EECs in the gastrointestinal (GI) tract play a crucial role in the control of food intake through several ways. Their activation results in the release of a large array of GI peptides, such as glucagon-like peptide 1 (GLP-1), cholecystokinin (CCK) and ghrelin,11 all involved in the control of food intake. Ghrelin, released by gastric EECs, is the only known orexigenic gut peptide and its plasma levels increase during fasting and rapidly fall after the meal.12,13 Other hormones, such as CCK, GLP 1, and peptide YY (PYY), exert an opposite effect, thus inhibiting food intake. CCK is the anorexigenic peptide that has been more thoroughly studied14; this hormone is released by I-cells into the duodenum in response to intra-luminal lipids and proteins.15,16 The CCK inhibitory effects on food intake are thought to be mediated via CCK1 receptors on vagal afferents.17
A previous study has shown that the stimulation of T2R with bitter compounds induced the release of the anorexigenic hormone CCK from mouse EEC lines.18 Moreover, the intragastric gavage of bitter compounds induced the release of ghrelin in mice.19 However, this evidence derives from experiments carried out in vitro or on animals, but no study has translated these findings to in vivo human models to define the role of the stimulation of these receptors on food intake.
In the wake of these results, we hypothesized that bitter taste receptors in the gut could be involved in the control of food intake. Our study thus aimed to investigate the effect of intra-luminal release of a bitter compound (HCl quinine) on food intake and gut hormone release in healthy subjects.
Twenty healthy adult volunteers (12 women, 8 men; median age 27 years) were recruited from the personnel and students of the Federico II University Medical School. Their mean ± SD body mass index (BMI) was 24 ± 4 kg/m2. Exclusion criteria were: prior abdominal surgery ? except for appendectomy, positive history of organic or functional GI diseases, use of medications able to affect GI motility and binge eating disorders. Informed written consent was obtained from all the participants and the study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Federico II University of Naples.
This was a randomized, double blind, placebo-controlled, cross-over study. Following an overnight fast, approximately at 11:30 AM, all subjects underwent
In addition, 8 out of 20 subjects also underwent 2 sessions of breath test for gastric emptying study. A standard meal was served to the subjects 60 minutes after the random administration of a capsule containing 18 mg of HCl quinine or placebo. Breath samples were collected during the experiments. After 1- week, the same subjects repeated the emptying study receiving a capsule containing HCl quinine or placebo, respectively. Similarly, randomization was assured by the computer generated random list.
A standardized test was used to assess calorie intake.22 A standard buffet meal was served 60 minutes after capsule administration. The standard meal was composed of white bread, cheese, and ham spread (Spunt?, Kraft Foods, Italy). The meal was administered by single portions, containing 89 kcal each, until maximum satiety was reached. The composition of single portions was the same: 50% carbohydrate, 31% fat, 19% protein. The food was served in excess to decrease the awareness of food intake. The quantity of food eaten, the volume of water drunk and the meal duration were recorded at the end of the test.
GI sensations were evaluated immediately before the administration of the capsule (
To determine PTC tasting status we invited all subjects to place, consecutively, 2 paper strips on their tongue: the first one was a control strip, whereas the second one was a strip of filter paper impregnated with 0.3 mg of PTC (Online science mall; Florida, USA). “Tasters” were defined as subjects who perceived the bitter taste when PTC-impregnated blotting paper strip was placed on the tongue. Furthermore, each subject was asked to rate the intensity of their bitter perception of PTC strip on a VAS ranging from 0 mm (no taste) to 100 mm (extremely strong taste).
Out of 20 subjects who participated in ad libitum test, 8 (4 women, median age 25 years) underwent a breath test study to determine gastric emptying in a randomized, double blind, placebo-controlled cross-over fashion. Sixty minutes after the administration of HCl quinine or placebo, they were asked to consume a test meal consisting of 60 g white bread, 10 g butter, 50 g ham, an omelet made from 1 egg with egg yolk charged with 74 kBq 13C-octanoic acid (Euriso-top, Saint-Aubin, France) and 100 mL water. The test meal contained 480 kcal (19% protein, 53% carbohydrate, and 31% fat). Subjects were encouraged to eat the meal within 10 minutes. Breath samples were collected at 15-minute intervals for 240 minutes postprandially. 13CO2-excretion in breath was subsequently analyzed using isotope-selective infrared spectroscopy to derive gastric emptying half-time.
Plasma samples were obtained at 0-60-90 minutes after capsules administration, placed in centrifuge tubes containing aprotinin and stored at ?80°C immediately after centrifugation. Total plasma immunoreactive ghrelin and CCK were measured by enzyme immunoassay. Ghrelin was measured in duplicate using commercial ELISA kits (Phoenix Pharmaceuticals)25; the inter- and intra-assay coefficients of variance were < 10%. The lower and upper detection limits for this assay were 0.12 ng/mL and 100 ng/mL. CCK (26?33 octapeptide non-sulfated form) was measured in duplicate using a commercial ELISA kit (Phoenix Pharmaceuticals)26; the inter- and intra-assay coefficients of variance were < 10%, with a lower detection limit of 0.04 ng/mL.
Preliminary data showed a differences of about 100 kcal in terms of calorie intake between the 2 sessions of the study with a standard deviation of differences of 120 kcal. Based on this data we calculated that a sample size of at least 13 patients would be required to test our hypothesis with a power of 0.80 and alpha level of 0.05 (PS Power and Sample Size Calculations; Version 3.0).
To evaluate significant differences in terms of GI sensations, a paired
Hormone profiles were evaluated both in terms of absolute values and as difference of T90 values with T0 (before capsule administration, ΔT90 vs T0) and T60 values (before test meal, ΔT90 vs T60). Between 2 sessions, ANOVA with repeated measures was used to evaluate differences of absolute values, whereas a paired
The statistical analysis was performed by the statistical software package SPSS for Windows Version 12.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). The results are reported as mean ± SD. Differences were considered statistically significant when
No subject experienced any oral bitter or unpleasant perception after HCl quinine or placebo administration, nor were there any adverse reactions after the experiment. No significant differences were observed between the 2 experiments in terms of satiety and desire to eat at T0, T60, and T end (Fig. 2). No significant differences were found regarding other symptoms or GI sensations (data not shown).
As shown in Figure 3, subjects ingested a significantly lower amount of calories when receiving the capsule containing HCl quinine than placebo (514 ± 248 vs 596 ± 286 kcal;
There were no significant differences in CCK absolute values when subjects received HCl quinine or placebo at the different time points (T0: 1.05 ± 0.57 vs 1.50 ± 1.11 ng/mL,
On the contrary, ghrelin plasma levels were not significantly different with HCl quinine or placebo, either when considered as absolute concentrations (T0: 2.48 ± 0.66 vs 2.57 ± 0.52 ng/mL,
The evaluation of gastric emptying revealed that in a subset of 8 subjects (5 tasters) HCl quinine was not able to significantly modify the rate of emptying in comparison to placebo (87 ± 14 vs 88 ± 12 minutes;
According to the PTC paper test, 11 out of 20 subjects were identified as “tasters,” while 9 subjects were not able to identify any bitter sensation and were classified as “non-tasters.”
Most interestingly, a further analysis revealed that PTC tasters ingested a significantly lower amount of calories when they received HCl quinine compared to placebo (526 ± 275 vs 659 ± 320 kcal;
Moreover, the linear regression showed a negative association between PTC bitter intensity and ΔKcal (
Regarding CCK release, in PTC tasters we observed that ΔT90 vs T0 and ΔT90 vs T60 after the administration of HCl quinine was higher compared to placebo but these differences were not statistically significant (ΔT90 vs T0: 0.84 ± 0.72 vs 0.12 ± 0.88 ng/mL,
The gut “senses” the food, although the molecular mechanisms of GI chemosensitivity are not fully understood. This study showed for the first time that the direct intra-luminal administration of a bitter taste receptor agonist is able to significantly reduce calorie intake in healthy subjects, likely acting on gut bitter taste receptors and altering gut hormone levels.
In our hands, the administration of HCl quinine in healthy subjects was able to significantly reduce calorie intake in an
We used quinine, an anti-malarial drug extracted from the bark of the cinchona tree, able to trigger bitter taste by the activation of several members of T2R family receptors.29 The drug was administered by an acid-resistant capsule for 2 main reasons: (1) in order to prevent the activation of the taste receptors of the oral cavity and (2) to prevent the release of ghrelin, produced by the X/A-like cells of the oxyntic glands of the stomach and to allow its release into the duodenum where CCK-releasing EECs are located.19
The role of bitter taste in the control of digestive functions is controversial: bitter compounds are naturally unpleasant. From a classical point of view, the bitter taste evolved in order to refuse potentially toxic compounds, such as plant alkaloids or microbial toxins. However, bitter compounds are also used to stimulate appetite or digestion in several parts of the world. For example, the habit of drinking aperitif before a meal is justified by the belief that the bitter taste stimulates appetite and increases gastric secretion.
Gustatory signals likely play a role in the cephalic phase of food intake to prepare the digestive tract to receive food.30 However, previous studies have shown that the activation of intestinal T2R results in the release of gut peptides, suggesting a role in the post-ingestion phase of food intake.18,19,31 In particular, the release of ghrelin from gastric EECs may explain, at least partially, that bitter compounds could increase appetite feeling.
The process that limits calorie intake is the result of a coordinated series of neural and humoral signals that originate from the gut. In particular, hormonal signaling is strongly influenced by intra-luminal chemicals (lipid, carbohydrates, and amino acids).32 According to our preliminary hypothesis, quinine failed to affect ghrelin release while significant postprandial CCK increase occurred. These data could explain, at least partially, the lower calorie intake in comparison to placebo. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first report indicating that quinine, a T2R agonist, is able to modify postprandial CCK-release in humans.
Taking into account that the gastric motility may affect food intake,33 we studied in a subset of volunteers the effect of quinine on gastric emptying rate. Our results failed to find any significant change of quinine compared to placebo, further suggesting that the reduced calorie intake is independent of gastric motility. Although bitter compounds in rats were able to delay gastric emptying, our result is in agreement with a previous study showing that quinine did not affect the rate of gastric emptying in humans.34,35
The sensitivity to bitter compounds is genetically grounded and individuals can be classified as “tasters” or “non-tasters” because of their ability to recognize PTC. Chang et al36 have demonstrated a positive relationship between PTC/PROP taster status and oral taste sensitivity to sucrose or quinine. To verify that both oral and gut taste mechanisms are comparable, we also screened the oral taste status of PTC in each subject. We found that the effects of quinine on calorie intake affected only subjects who were able to discriminate PTC, but not PTC non-tasters. However, only a non-significant trend in CCK increase was observed after the administration of quinine in PTC tasters.
Our results suggest that quinine-mediated CCK release is likely unable
Various studies have tried to elucidate the physiological role of gut taste receptors in humans. Gerspach et al37 demonstrated that lactisole, a T1R1/T1R3 (sweet) antagonist, induced a significant reduction in GLP-1 and PYY but not CCK secretion, in response to intragastric and intraduodenal glucose administration. Our study demonstrates, for the first time, that bitter taste receptors are also involved in the physiological mechanisms that control appetite in humans. Therefore, while the role of the T2Rs in the oral cavity would prevent the intake of hazardous chemicals, the bitter taste receptors in the GI tract may act as a second level mechanism which is able to further limit the intake of potentially toxic bitter chemicals. Jeon et al31 showed that T2R signaling stimulates the secretion of CCK from EECs and induces the expression of ATP-binding cassette B1, a transporter expressed on the apical membrane of intestinal epithelial cells able to limit absorption of toxic substrates, both in Caco-2 cells and mouse intestine, through a CCK-dependent mechanism.
Summarizing, we showed that the intra-luminal release of quinine, a bitter taste receptor agonist, significantly reduces the calorie intake in an